Animal Experimentation: A Continued Necessity in Medical Research

Katie Loughman, Kathryn Brebner, Emily Mei 

Introduction

World War II is often referred to as the bloodiest war in history. From 1939 to 1945, over 50 million soldiers and civilians lost their lives to bullets, bombs, lethal gases, or the horrifying and systematic genocide of the Jewish people (History.com, n.d.). Families were torn apart by loss both on and off the battlefield. But what most fail to realize is that many of these deaths were not caused by the weapons themselves, but instead by the bacteria that colonized the wounds that they inflicted. In the midst of this devastating period of history, there was a beacon of hope: the development of penicillin, which is still regarded today as the greatest achievement in medical history. Continue Reading

The Benefits of Spaying and Neutering Domesticated Cats and Dogs

Abby Taylor, Animal Science

Trish Vosburg, Wildlife Ecology and Conservation

Emily Sgarlat, Natural Resources Conservation

Human/Animal Relationships

Spaying and neutering saves lives. In her first year a female cat can have up to 3 litters, this could mean an average of 12 kittens per year. In her second year if she and each of her kittens have three litters, that’s 144 offspring… if this cycle continues, in 4 years a single cat and her kittens can be responsible for 10,736 offspring. (Pet Health Network, 2011) There is an overpopulation of domestic cats and dogs in the United States. There are not enough homes for these animals and many of them end up in shelters. Due to overcrowding of shelters approximately 600 dogs, puppies, cats, and kittens are killed every hour (AHeinz57, 2015). The role domesticated cats and dogs play in our lives is often as company, we choose to take on the financial responsibility that comes with owning a cat or dog because they add happiness to our lives. Part of this financial responsibility includes spaying or neutering these animal companions.

Continue Reading

The Use of Animal Models in Medical Research is Superior to Non-Animal Alternatives

 

Lab Animal Model [Untiled image of a lab mouse in gloved hand] Retrieved April 7, 2015 from http://www3.imperial.ac.uk/newsandeventspggrp/imperialcollege/newssummary/news_9-12-2013-18-21-18

Lab Animal Model
[Untiled image of a lab mouse in gloved hand] Retrieved April 7, 2015 from http://www3.imperial.ac.uk/newsandeventspggrp/imperialcollege/newssummary/news_9-12-2013-18-21-18

Alyssa Tonelli – Animal Science

Christine O’Brien – Animal Science

Dan Garrigan – Animal Science

“That’s funny,” he thought as he grabbed one of the Petri dishes that had not been in contact with the Lysol tray. He noticed that there were separate colonies of staphylococci on the Petri dish. Toward the edge, he noted a colony of mold approximately 20 mm in diameter (Ligon, 2004). He did not see any staphylococci around the mold.

The Discovery of Penicillin

In the year 1928, scientist Alexander Fleming made one of the biggest medical breakthroughs ever recorded – the discovery of penicillin and its antibacterial properties. Some may classify this discovery “accidental,” however, this does not acknowledge its sheer importance to the medical field. Fleming’s experiment demonstrated that penicillin’s bacteriostatic and bactericidal properties, meaning that it would not only kill microbes but inhibit future microbial growth (Ligon, 2004). However, Fleming was unsure of how to apply this discovery to humans. In the war-stricken 1930’s, thousands of people were sick and dying due to septic infections that weren’t easily treatable in the field. Medics needed an easy and effective way to prevent infected wounds from infiltrating the rest of the body. The need for penicillin in a pharmaceutical capacity became critical. (Freedman et. al, 1981). Luckily Ernst Chain, Howard Florey, and their team of research assistants were up for the challenge. Continue Reading

Naturalistic Housing and Environmental Enrichment for Nonhuman Primates

Wisconsin National Primate Research Center [Photograph]. (2014). Retrieved November 12, 2014, from: http://wisconsinwatch.org/2014/07/university-of-wisconsin-to-reprise-controversial-monkey-studies/

Wisconsin National Primate Research Center [Photograph]. (2014). Retrieved November 12, 2014, from: http://wisconsinwatch.org/2014/07/university-of-wisconsin-to-reprise-controversial-monkey-studies/

Introduction

Research laboratories around the world utilize animals as their subjects in hopes of making scientific discoveries that will benefit humans. However, in exchange, the welfare of these animal subjects becomes a concern, especially that of non-human primates. In the last year, the poor welfare of these primates has been exposed, as Harvard Medical School announced it’s plans to close down their primate research facility. They came to this decision as a result of multiple citations issued for incidents occurring from 2011-2012. Although they stated that their decision to shut down was based on financial hardships, a string of incidents resulting in multiple primate fatalities suggests there was more that led to that decision (Valdmanis, 2013). Valdmanis (2013) states that there were different causes to the death of four primates, including an overdose of anesthetic that led to liver failure in one, dehydration in two others, and one that suffered accidental strangulation by a chain attached to a toy. Based on this sequence of isolated events, the welfare of non-human primates in laboratory settings is a concern, as multiple deaths have occurred due to neglect. If primate research laboratories continue to operate in their current condition, this neglect will continue. If these conditions are not made known to those who work in these laboratories, the same careless mistakes will continue to be made. While there are a number of regulations in place in regards to the treatment of primates, the next step towards bettering their welfare seems to be environmental enrichment.

Causal Analysis

The root of the concerns with the welfare of non-human primates stems from the lack of environmental enrichment in the laboratory settings, which allows for species-typical behavior such as perching, grooming, foraging, and toy manipulation (V. Reinhardt; A. Reinhardt, 2008). Beaver (1989) notes that this lack of enrichment leads to depression, aggression, and self-mutilation among the primates. Since many laboratories are shutting down based on the welfare concerns of primates, research on primates altogether may come to a stop. Primate research is important because this species is the most closely related species to humans, which means that utilizing them for research will be beneficial to finding cures for diseases that affect the human population (Gabarini, 2010). In addition to this, experimental findings that are skewed, resulting from decreased welfare, could have negative impacts on humans that are using the drugs developed through the research (Shively, Clarkson & Kaplan, 1989). Shively et al. (1989) described a significant increase in the incidence of coronary artery atherosclerosis in female cynomolgus monkey housed alone, this underlying physiological occurrence holds the potential to skew reactions to testing and medications. Therefore, for better treatment of non-human primates, a laboratory setting should be created that mimics their natural habitat and allows for them to practice their natural behaviors.

Social Enrichment

Since non-human primates naturally live in social groups, housing them in close proximity to one another ensures social contact and avoids the development of abnormal behaviors. For example, Thom and Crockett (2008) propose placing animals in cages with a grooming contact bar panel, allowing social grooming to occur. The authors also state that the social needs of these animals can be met if they are housed together in large cages, connected “run-through” cages, or in cages connected by tunnels that allow for physical contact.  Beaver (1989) states that a study placing non-human primates in isolation identified development of abnormal behaviors, which included body clasping, rocking, hyperaggression, rigid stances, sexual dysfunction, clinging, repetitive circling, and self-mutilation (p. 7). These findings exemplify the critical importance of ensuring that non-human primates are housed in a setting that allows social contact.

Further research is being conducted on how to provide primates with the opportunity for social interactions while still providing scientists the opportunity to work with primates on an individual basis to obtain valid results and measurements. A social tethering system allows for scientific testing to take place while still “[p]rovid[ing] primates with the opportunity to engage in species typical behavior and thereby minimiz[ing] conditions that have been identified as contributing to the development of abnormal behaviors associated with individual housing” (Coelho, 1990, p. 388). This system still effectively allows scientist to sample body fluids, monitor physiological parameters, collect urine, monitor food/water consumption, and monitor their physical activity and social interactions. The Coelho (1990) system consisted of a specialized cage with a section for housing small social groups of adult baboons, and another section consisting of the tether and indwelling catheter system to collect samples. This system allowed for up to four baboons to be housed together, and this number could vary depending on the size of the housing unit created. The units themselves had the opportunity to be subdivided by means of a wire fence. “The tether system consisted of a backpack, a cloth jacket, a stainless-steel flexible cable containing electrical cable and catheters, and a saline infusion pump mounted on the top of the cage”(Coelho, 1990, pp. 390). Depending on the specific type of research is going to be conducted with the primates, this system could provide an excellent model for research housing.

Enrichment with Wood

In addition to the importance of providing social conditions to research primates, providing other components that simulate a natural habitat, such as the presence of wood and the opportunity for foraging, is vital to increase welfare. For most primates, species-typical behaviors include perching, climbing, and foraging for resources such as food. The United States Department of Agriculture requires that animals held in captivity be provided enrichment that allows the expression of these activities (United States Department of Agriculture, 1991). The opportunity for primates to engage in many species-typical behaviors can be provided through wooden stimuli. Reinhardt (2000) states, “wooden objects provide inexpensive, safe, long-term and effective stimulation for the expression of non-injurious, species-typical behaviors such as perching, gnawing, gouging, manipulating and playing” (p. 13). Furthermore, there is a tendency for wood to warp over time, and this provides primates with continual intellectual stimulation (Luchins, Baker, Gilbert, Blanchard, & Rudolf, 2011).

The benefits to providing wooden enrichment expand beyond it’s inexpensive cost, low hazard threat, and strong promotion of species-typical behavior. For example, Simonson (2002) states that using hydroscopic materials, such as wood, increases indoor comfort and air quality in comparison to similar structures made of steel and concrete. This statement validates their research by saying in their tests, “…as many as 10 or more people of 100 are satisfied with the thermal comfort conditions (warm respiratory conditions) at the end of occupation.” Similarly the authors determined occupant discomfort was reduced by 25%(Simonson, 2002, abstract). Given the close relationship between humans and non-human primates it seems fair to say that they should be housed in a comfortable environment. It is important to create a comfortable environment for nonhuman primates to create accurate test results. This is comparable to taking a test in a hot and humid room. The heat makes it difficult to focus, but the humidity makes it that much worse, not to mention participants are unlikely to perform to the same standard as they might in less humid conditions.

 

Opposition to Enrichment

Some opposition to naturalistic habitats in a research or laboratory setting has been presented from researchers concerned over the potential of wood to present as a fomite. If this were the case, wooden objects, such as perches and toys, would need to be replaced frequently to prevent potential disease transfer, which would pose an expense on researchers (Bayne, Dexter, Hurst, Strange & Hill, 1993). This concern has been invalidated by research done on the sanitation of wood. After researchers sanitized three different sources of primate cages, they found that Manzanita wood displayed on 13% growth of gram-negative bacteria compared to a 50% growth rate on plastic toys and 32% growth on the actual cages (Luchins et al., 2011, p. 6). This indicates that wood will not pose threat as a potential fomite and that it’s easy sterilization makes wood an excellent candidate for a source of environmental enrichment. Reinhardt (1997) expands on this by showing that with wooden objects being sanitized in the same routine as cage sanitation procedures there has been no incidence of health risks or injuries associated with wood.

Opposition to Primate Research

Opposition to research involving animals in any form will hold that the shutting down of research laboratories is a cause for celebration. However, in terms of human medicine, this is not the case. The utilization of primates in research holds a critical importance to contemporary medicine (Gabarini, 2010). Incredible advances in the treatment of Parkinson’s diseases, Alzheimer’s disease, and AIDS have been made in the past decade, and these advances would have been impossible without the model of primates (Capitanio & Emborg, 2008). The understanding and treatment of alcoholism, sleep related disorders, and behavioral disorders are also results of the major advances made from research conducted on primates (Garbarini, 2010). In the contemporary world of medicine, primates serve a vital role in the treatment of human disease and disorder, and as long as steps are made to provide these primates the maximal care and welfare, scientists should be able to continue their advances. Applying a naturalistic environment should be the key to establishing this maximum welfare.

Conclusion

It is clear that there is a lack of enrichment in current research laboratories, leading to the decreased welfare of nonhuman primates. Enrichment is necessary not only on an ethical standpoint, but it is also required to successfully conduct primate research and obtain the most valid results. Enrichment provided through the use of housing that allows for social contact and naturalistic elements such as wooden toys and foraging stimuli simulates a natural environment at which primates function best. This natural environment should promote species-typical behaviors and decrease the incidence of depression, agitation, self-mutilation and stereotypies amongst primates. This will correspond to an increase in primate welfare, which will allow breakthroughs in modern medicine to continue. Currently, the American Association for Laboratory Animal Sciences regulates and audits laboratories through the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC). While the IACUC enforces strict regulations in terms of cage height, sanitation, and food availability, there are not specific guidelines in terms of enrichment (Garber et al., 2011). Hopefully, by reviewing the information presented in this paper and other research, the IACUC will be influenced to mandate policies that promote enrichment and increase the welfare of non-human primates used in research.

References

Bayne K.A., Dexter S.L., Hurst J.K., Strange G.M., Hill E.E. (1993). Kong toys for laboratory primates: are they really an enrichment or just fomites? Lab Anim Sci, 43, 78–85. Retreived from PubMed.

Beaver, B. (1989). Environmental enrichment for laboratory animals. ILAR Journal, 31(2), 5-11. doi: http://ilarjournal.oxfordjournals.org/content/31/2/5.full

Coelho A.M. Jr., and Carey K.D (1990). A social tethering system for nonhuman primates used in laboratory research. Lab Anim Sci, 40(4), 388-394. Retrieved from PubMed.

Eckert, K., Niemeyer, C., Anonymous , Rogers, R.W., Seier, J., Ingersoll, B., Barklay, L., Brinkman, C., Oliver, S., Buckmaster, C., Knowles, L., Pyle ,S. (2000). Wooden objects for enrichment: A discussion. Laboratory Primate Newsletter 39(3), 1-4.

Garbarini, N. (2010). Primates as a model for research. Disease Models & Mechanisms, 3 (2), 15-19. doi:10.1242 Harvard Fined $24,000 for Animal Mistreatment After Monkeys Die.” Reuters. N.p., 18 Dec 2013.

Garber, C., Barbee, R., Bielitzki, J.,Clayton, L., Donovan, J., Kohn, D., Lipman, N., Locke, P., Melcher, J., Quimby, F., Turner, P., Wood, G., Wurbel, H. (2011). Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. National Research Council, 8. Retrieved from American Association of Laboratory Animal Sciences.

Luchins, K., Baker, K., Gilbert, M., Blanchard, J., & Rudolf, B. (2011). Manzanita wood: A sanitizable enrichment option for nonhuman primates. Journal of the American Association Laboratory Animal Science, 50 (6), 884–887. Retrieved from PubMed.

Shively, C. A., Clarkson, T. B., & Kaplan, J. R. (1989). Social deprivation and coronary artery atherosclerosis in female cynomolgus monkeys. Atherosclerosis, 77, 69-76.

Simonson, C. J., Salonvaara, M., & Ojanen, T. (2002). The effect of structures on indoor humidity possibility to improve comfort and perceived air quality. Indoor Air, 12(4), 243-251.

Thom, J., & Crockett, C. (2008). Managing environmental enhancement plans for individual research projects at a national primate research center. Journal of the American Association Laboratory Animal Science, 47 (3), 51–57. Retrieved from PubMed.

Reinhardt, V. and Reinhardt, A. (2008). Environmental enrichment and refinement for nonhuman primates kept in research laboratories: a photographic documentation and literature review. Washington (DC): Animal Welfare Institute. 1-10.

Reinhardt, V., Houser, W.D., Cowley, D., Champoux, M. (1987). Preliminary comments on environmental enrichment with branches for individually caged rhesus monkeys. Labrotary Primate News, 26, 1–3.

United States Department of Agriculture (1991). Animal Welfare; Standards; Final Rule. Federal Register 56(32), 6426-6505.

Valdmanis, R. (2013, December 18). Harvard fined $24,000 for animal mistreatment after monkeys die. Retrieved December 3, 2014, from http://www.reuters.com/article/2013/12/18/us-usa-harvard-monkeys-idUSBRE9BH11120131218

 

 

The Truth Behind the Curtain: Banning Performing Circus Elephants

Introduction

Most people have that one friend that they can go weeks or even months without seeing, and when they are reunited it is as if no amount of time has gone by. Now imagine going twenty two years without seeing that friend. With so much time passed, it is safe to say things would not be the same between you. However, this was not the case for two old friends, Shirley and Jenny, a pair of retired performing Asian elephants. Continue Reading

The Detriments of Captive Cetacean Research

 

Orca whale in the wild (Lacz)

Orca whale in the wild (Lacz)

In January of 2013, at the Sundance Film Festival, hundreds of people watched in horror and suspense as the truth about marine parks was revealed in Gabriela Cowperthwaithe’s documentary, Blackfish. The once jovial picture of killer whales and dolphins performing amazing tricks at parks like SeaWorld was dramatically tainted to an image of their imprisonment, mental torment, and the dire consequences of it all. This documentary sparked a great deal of controversy over the issue of keeping cetaceans in captivity, a multi-million dollar industry that’s been a source of entertainment since the mid-1960’s. Many viewers empathized wholeheartedly with the film, while others believed it was nothing but fabricated lies. An opinion piece like Blackfish allows room for this sort of debate; however, scientific literature provides concrete evidence that keeping cetaceans in captivity for research or entertainment purposes is mercilessly and unnecessarily taxing on their physical and emotional welfare, and provides little to no conclusive data for practical human use.  Although the whales portrayed in Blackfish are confined for entertainment purposes, other cetaceans held for research are kept in similar enclosures, meaning they will have similar mental and physical reactions to the captivity.

Continue Reading